Cracking the code of animal boredom

Wednesday 7 May 2025

At 29, Dr Morgan Heslop has tackled a question that many of us may never have considered — can animals get bored? And if they do, how would we even know?

Dr Morgan Heslop.

Born and raised in Tauranga, and now living in the Bay of Islands, Dr Heslop graduated with her PhD from Te Kunenga ki Pūrehuroa Massey University this week. For her doctoral research, she investigated an underexplored topic in animal welfare science: boredom.

“My thesis explores what boredom might be like for animals. The tricky thing is, unlike with people, we can’t just ask animals how they feel, so we have to get creative and scientific about recognising their emotions.”

Dr Heslop says people often assume they can read animal emotions easily — like knowing when your dog is excited for a walk or your cat is angling for dinner. But she cautions that our intuitions don’t always line up with reality.

“A classic example is when a dog pulls its lips back and looks like it's smiling — people think it’s happy, but it might actually be scared. If we misinterpret signals like this, it can lead to dangerous or stressful situations. It’s no different with boredom — we might think we’re giving an animal everything it needs, when in fact, it could be struggling in silence.”

Unlike emotions such as pain or hunger, which have clear biological indicators, boredom doesn’t stem from physical discomfort. It’s a complicated emotional state linked to things like cognition, motivation, attention and meaning — concepts that are difficult to measure, especially in animals.

“We can’t measure boredom with a blood test or a stethoscope. We have to look at how animals behave over time — patterns, not isolated moments,” Dr Heslop explains.

Through her research, she argues that boredom isn’t about any one behaviour, like pacing or yawning, but rather about patterns of behaviour that indicate an animal is seeking to organise, change or support its mental workload.

“A bored animal might return to the same toy over and over, or get up and move around constantly, as if trying to find something to do. Recognising these patterns takes time and close observation, often more than we typically dedicate to assessing an animal’s emotional state.”

The inspiration for this work came during the early stages of the COVID-19 pandemic. Like many others, Dr Heslop found herself confined to her home, living a restricted and repetitive life, much like many animals do every day.

“I realised I was experiencing something that’s very normal for animals in human care — having no control over where I went, who I saw or what I did. I was bored, and it made me think: what if this is how animals feel all the time, and we just don’t see it?”

This idea led her to discover that animal boredom is a relatively new field, with only a handful of studies published globally. Seeing a gap in the science, and a chance to make a difference, she dove in.

Dr Heslop’s findings have the potential to change how we care for animals across a wide range of environments.

“Think of all the animals kept in cages, crates, kennels, tanks and stables. Their environments are often unchanging and restrictive, which are key ingredients for boredom. If boredom is widespread, it could be one of the most common and overlooked welfare issues today.”

What surprised her most during her research was how diverse people’s experiences of boredom are — something that further complicates understanding the emotion in animals.

“Some people find boredom draining and low-energy, while others describe it as agitating or even painful. And then some people say they never get bored at all! That really highlighted how diverse emotional experiences can be, and how careful we need to be when trying to interpret what another being — especially an animal — might be feeling.”

While her thesis was at the heart of her time at Massey, Dr Heslop made the most of every opportunity around her. She took part in the Three Minute Thesis competition, represented students on the Human Ethics Committee, supported peers in their own research and taught both undergraduate and postgraduate students.

“But the most important part of my time at Massey was the people. I had an incredible team of supervisors, and I made life-long friends. When things got tough — and they always do in research — having people who understand, support you and can make you laugh makes all the difference.”

Now, with her PhD completed, she is working as a Research Officer at Massey, a role she’ll hold until the end of the year before heading overseas.

“I’m hoping to keep working in animal welfare — to take the skills and knowledge I’ve developed and apply them in practical ways that improve the lives of animals. There’s still so much we don’t understand, and I want to be part of changing that.”

Related news

Research explores physiological effects of dietary water in cats

Monday 27 November 2023

For her doctoral research, Dr Margreet Hekman studied cats fed high and low moisture diets. She found that low moisture diets led to lower water intake, higher urine concentration and higher after-meal plasma and vasopressin concentrations.

Technology reveals the hidden world of an endangered subantarctic penguin population

Tuesday 13 June 2023

Dr Chris Muller’s PhD research saw him living on an isolated subantarctic island and developing new drone technology to gain insight into one of the world’s rarest and most endangered penguin species.

Saving endangered bird species a passion for PhD graduate

Wednesday 24 May 2023

For his doctoral research, Dr Enzo Reyes investigated the factors that need to be considered to successfully translocate the endangered Floreana mockingbird to Floreana Island in the Galápagos Archipelago.

PhD graduate investigates the welfare of horses used for hands-on teaching

Friday 25 November 2022

Dr Lauréline Guinnefollau’s thesis looked at the impact that hands-on educational activities have on Massey’s population of teaching horses.